U.S. Department of Transportation
Federal Highway Administration
1200 New Jersey Avenue, SE
Washington, DC 20590
202-366-4000


Skip to content
Facebook iconYouTube iconTwitter iconFlickr iconLinkedInInstagram

Federal Highway Administration Research and Technology
Coordinating, Developing, and Delivering Highway Transportation Innovations

Report
This report is an archived publication and may contain dated technical, contact, and link information
Publication Number: FHWA-HRT-08-070
Date: October 2008

Test Track and Driving Simulator Evaluations of Warnings to Prevent Right-Angle Crashes at Signalized Intersections

PDF Version (460 KB)

PDF files can be viewed with the Acrobat® Reader®

FOREWORD

The Federal Highway Administration (FHWA) Office of Safety Research and Development (R&D) is focused on improving highway operations and safety by increasing the knowledge and understanding of the effects of intersection design on operational efficiency and safety. Intelligent Transportation Systems (ITS) have been shown to have both safety and operational benefits. ITS is a worldwide initiative to incorporate communications and information technology in transportation systems. This study was conducted to investigate the potential for an ITS countermeasure to reduce the number of collisions that result from red-light violations at signalized intersections. In the United States, red-light violations result in about 1,200 fatalities or incapacitating injuries each year. At intersections where it is installed, a system that detects violations as they are about to occur and then warns all drivers who are at risk of a consequent collision could reduce the frequency of this type of fatal crash by as much as 88 percent. This research examined one of the assumptions underlying the estimated 88-percent crash reduction.

Michael F. Trentacoste

Director

Office of Safety

Research and Development

Notice

This document is disseminated under the sponsorship of the U.S. Department of Transportation in the interest of information exchange. The U.S. Government assumes no liability for the use of the information contained in this document.

The U.S. Government does not endorse products or manufacturers. Trademarks or manufacturers' names appear in this report only because they are considered essential to the objective of the document.

Quality Assurance Statement

The Federal Highway Administration (FHWA) provides high-quality information to serve Government, industry, and the public in a manner that promotes public understanding. Standards and policies are used to ensure and maximize the quality, objectivity, utility, and integrity of its information. FHWA periodically reviews quality issues and adjusts its programs and processes to ensure continuous quality improvement.

Technical Report Documentation Page

1. Report No.
FHWA-HRT-08-070

2. Government Accession No.

3. Recipient Catalog No.

4. Title and Subtitle
Test Track and Driving Simulator Evaluations of Warnings to Prevent Right-Angle Crashes at Signalized Intersections

5. Report Date
October 2008

6. Performing Organization Code

7. Author(s)
Vaughan W. Inman, Gregory W. Davis, Ihab El-Shawarby, and Hesham Rakha

8. Performing Organization Report No.

9. Performing Organization Name and Address
FHWA Office of Safety R&D, Human Centered Systems Team
Federal Highway Administration
Turner Fairbank Highway Research Center
6300 Georgetown Pike
McLean, VA 22101

10. Work Unit No. (TRAIS)

11. Contract or Grant No.
DTFH61-02-C-00064

12. Sponsoring Agency Name and Address
Office of Operations Research, Development and Technology
Federal Highway Administration
Turner Fairbank Highway Research Center
6300 Georgetown Pike
McLean, VA 22101

13. Type of Report and Period Covered
Final Report, Oct 2002–May 2006

14. Sponsoring Agency Code

15. Supplementary Notes
This study was conducted by the FHWA Office of Safety R&D, Human Centered Systems Team. Funding for this research came from the U.S. DOT Joint Program Office. SAIC designed the research, analyzed the data, and prepared this report. AAI Engineering Support developed the simulation scenarios for Experiment 1 and ran the driving simulator. The Virginia Tech Transportation Institute collected data on the Virginia Smart Road and participated in analysis and reporting of that data. The research was performed for the FHWA Office of Operations R&D and the ITS Joint Program Office. Paul J. Tremont was the FHWA Task Manager and Thomas Granda was the FHWA contract manager.

16. Abstract
Two experiments (simulator and test track) were conducted to validate the concept of a system designed to warn potential victims of a likely red-light violator. The warning system uses sensors to detect vehicles that are unlikely to stop at red traffic signals and uses signs and flashing lights to warn drivers who might collide with a violator. Several human factors issues need to be addressed before such a system could be deployed. The experiments for this study addressed one of these issues—whether, if warned, a sufficient number of drivers would respond in a way that would allow them to avoid a right-angle collision. The results suggest that in the case where no other vehicles precede or follow, a majority of drivers who receive a conspicuous warning will act by braking sharply. Driver responses in both tests were similar. The test track results support the continued use of driving simulators in development of the system. Further research is required to assess responses to warnings given to drivers within a stream of traffic.

17. Key Words
Signalized intersection; red-light violation; warning; CICAS; perception-response time; intersection collision avoidance; mid-phase warning; vehicle-infrastructure integration; VII

18. Distribution Statement
No restrictions. This document is available to the public through the National Technical Information Service, Springfield, VA 22161.

19. Security Classif. (of this report)
Unclassified

20. Security Classif. (of this page)
Unclassified

21. No. of Pages
46

22. Price

Form DOT F 1700.7 (8-72) Reproduction of completed page authorized


SI* (Modern Metric) Conversion Factors


Table of Contents

List of Figures

List of Tables

Executive Summary

The two experiments reported here were conducted to evaluate the feasibility of warning drivers who are at risk of a collision with a red-light violator. There are approximately 1,200 fatalities yearly as a result of right-angle collisions between red-light violators and vehicles that have the right of way. In the first of two experiments, a driving simulator was used to assess three alternative infrastructure-based victim warning configurations against a control configuration. The second of these experiments used a test track to validate one of the warnings that was simulated in the first experiment.

The warnings evaluated in the simulator were (1) a half-second amber followed by a red signal, (2) an immediate change from a green signal to a red that was supplemented by red wig-wag signals and an electronic stop sign symbol and simulated strobe lights (all mounted on the traffic signal mast arm), and (3) mast arm warnings plus the onset of simulated in-pavement red-light-emitting diodes and simulated "intelligent" rumble strips. Driver responses to the three warning conditions were compared to responses in the control condition, the onset of a 4-s amber signal. Two-hundred and two drivers participated in the study, with approximately 50 drivers in each of the four conditions.

Warnings were deemed to be sufficient to prevent a collision with a red-light violator if the arrival of the participant at the intersection was delayed by at least 1 s, per the arrival time that would be expected if they continued at the speed they were traveling when the warning was initiated. Based on the 1-s delay criterion, the most conspicuous warning, which included in-pavement lights and intelligent rumble strips, theoretically prevented 67 percent of the potential collisions. That warning was more than twice as effective in preventing potential collisions as the control condition (4-s amber).

A closed-road test track experiment was conducted to validate the findings from the driving simulation. The test track warning included an immediate red signal, wig-wag red lights, an electronic stop sign symbol, and strobe lights. Sixty drivers participated in the test track experiment. Half of these received the warning when 55 m (180 ft) from the intersection stop line, and half received the warning when they were 32 m (105 ft) from the intersection stop line. Ninety percent of the drivers who received the warning at 55 m (180 ft) stopped within 5 m (17 ft) of the stop line, while only one driver who received the warning at 32 m (105 ft) from the stop line stopped. The decelerations of the drivers that stopped for the warning were similar to those observed in the simulator.

This study provides evidence that conspicuous infrastructure warnings might be effective in preventing collisions with red-light violators. In this study, no other vehicles were traveling either ahead or behind, and a majority of drivers braked aggressively for such a warning. However, important human factors questions remain pertaining to driver reactions to such warnings when other vehicles are ahead or behind and the effectiveness of infrastructure-based, vehicle-based, and combined infrastructure- and vehicle-based warnings. The Federal Highway Administration (FHWA) is currently conducting research to address these issues.

Introduction

Background

At signalized intersections, straight crossing-path crashes occur when a vehicle violates a red light and collides with a vehicle crossing from an approach that has the right-of-way. The consequences of straight crossing-path crashes at signalized intersections are serious. (1) In 2006, there were 1,197 fatalities in 1,058 collisions of this type in the United States.(2) Zimmerman and Bonneson report that the median time into the red phase of the violations that result in collisions is about 9 s. In their sample of 41 red-light violation collisions, time-into-red ranged from 0.6 to 44.2 s, with a mean of 14.1 s.(3) They suggest that the majority of red-light violation related collisions occur after queues have cleared. In this situation, both the signal violator and victim are likely to be traveling close to free-flow speed.

Ferlis described an infrastructure-based Intelligent Transportation System (ITS) countermeasure for reducing straight crossing-path crashes at signalized intersections.(4) He estimated that, where deployed, this infrastructure-based warning to both the violator and victim could reduce straight crossing-path collisions by as much as 88 percent. The Ferlis estimate was based on the assumptions that 80 percent of violators would respond if warned, and 40 percent of potential victims would respond if warned.

The present research tested the Ferlis assumption that 40 percent of potential victims who are warned would respond to avoid a right-angle collision. Two experiments are reported—one based in a driving simulator, and one conducted on a closed-road course.

Approach

Two experiments were designed to test the assumption that 40 percent of potential victims would respond to a red-light violation warning. The first experiment used a driving simulator to assess three alternative victim warning configurations and a control configuration. The second experiment used a closed-road course to validate one of the warnings that was simulated in the first experiment.

It is recognized that red-light violators can only be reliably detected when they are relatively close to an intersection. Ferlis suggested that a violator approaching an intersection at 56 k/h (35 mi/h) could be detected about 43 m (141 ft) from an intersection.(4) Because warnings to potential victims cannot occur earlier than detection of potential violators, it is possible that potential victims might be already committed to entering the intersection. Therefore, any victim warning needs to be compelling and immediately comprehensible. The warning system envisioned by Ferlis had three major elements: (1) sensors to detect the speed of vehicles approaching a red traffic signal, (2) an algorithm to compute the likelihood of an approaching vehicle to run a red light, and (3) a warning to potential violators and their potential victims. The experiments were designed to assess the probability that selected warnings would induce potential victims to slow or stop when the warnings were presented at a point on the intersection approach where they had already committed to proceeding through an intersection without stopping, regardless of whether or not the signal remained green.

Simulator Experiment

To assess the response to an unexpected warning, it is first necessary to create an expectation that the traffic signals are operating as usual. To build that expectation in the driving simulator, participants drove through 32 intersections before they were presented a warning. Upon approach to 25 of those 32 intersections, no slowing or stopping was required in response to signals that cycled back to green well before the participants needed to slow or stop, and none changed to amber when the participant was close to the intersection. Upon approach to the remaining four intersections, the signal changed from green, but the participant always had at least 168 m (550 ft) or 8.3 s to slow or stop. Thus, previous to the 33rd intersection, participants never needed to make a rapid decision in response to traffic signals. Furthermore, at the six intersections that immediately preceded the 33rd, the light turned green before the participant vehicle was within 122 m (400 ft) of the stop line. At the 33rd intersection, when they reached a trigger distance of either 55 m (180 ft) or 66 m (215 ft), participants were presented with one of the three warnings or with a 4-s amber (the control condition).

Test Track Experiment

To build an expectation of normal phase changes in the closed-road test track experiment, participants drove through the same intersection 24 times before a warning was presented. On 20 of the 24 intersection approaches before the warning was presented, a 4-s amber was presented. Some of those phase changes occurred when the participant was less than 2 s from the stop line. On the remaining four approaches, a green signal was presented. Thus, in the simulator the intended expectation at the warning intersection was that of a green light. In contrast, in the closed road the intended expectation at the warning intersection was a high probability of a 4-s amber signal. In both the simulation and in the field, the warning should have been unexpected.

Dilemma Zone Behavior

The dilemma zone, as defined here, is the area upstream of the stop line where drivers are inconsistent in stop-and-go decisions when confronted with the onset of an amber signal. Although the primary purpose of these studies was to examine driver response to unexpected warnings at signalized intersections, the 4-s amber presentations that occurred in proximity to the intersections also provided an opportunity to examine dilemma zone behavior.(5) The test track experiment findings with respect to dilemma-zone decisionmaking may be found in separate reports by El-Shawarby et al.cit_bf(6,7,8)However, responses to amber-signal onsets are summarized briefly in the Results section of this report.

Driving Simulation (Experiment 1)

Driving Simulator Characteristics

The simulator (as configured for this experiment) consisted of a late model compact car chassis that was mounted inside of a section of a cylindrical projection screen. The cylindrical screen had a vertical radius of 2.7 m (107 inches). When looking at the center of the projections, the driver' eye point was set 20 cm (8 inches) back from the center of the cylinder. An Electrohome Marquee 9500LC projected a 1,920 by 1,200 pixel image onto the screen. The image covered the driver' forward field-of-view 88 degrees horizontally 50 degrees vertically. A Silicon Graphics Infinte Reality 2 graphics subsystem generated the images at 60 Hz. The vehicle brake, accelerator, and steering wheel inputs were routed to a Silicon Graphics ONYX2 computer that hosted a vehicle dynamics model. The vehicle dynamics model updated the virtual world on a per frame basis. Key features of the vehicle dynamics model were as follows:

  • Six degree-of-freedom equations.
  • A simplified model of a four-speed automatic transmission.
  • Tire skid limits based on side friction and braking force.
  • Steering disturbances (to simulate side forces such as wind).
  • A 3-degree-of-freedom (tilt, roll, and heave) motion base that responded to vehicle dynamics model outputs.

Methods—Driving Simulation

Conditions Tested

Four signal conditions were tested in the simulator: three specially designed infrastructure-to-driver warning displays and a conventional 4-s amber phase that served as a control condition. The three potential warning displays varied in conspicuity and in the degree to which they differed from current infrastructure.

Four-Second Amber (Control)

The control signal condition was a 4-s amber that was initiated when the distance of the participant vehicle to the stop line became equal to or less than to a particular trigger distance. The warning trigger distance was either 55 m (180 ft) or 66 m (215 ft). The difference between control condition responses and warning condition responses was intended to provide a measure against which responses to the warnings could be compared.

Condition 1

This signal condition consisted of a short amber (0.5 s) followed by a red. This condition was included because it could be implemented easily at low cost by using existing infrastructure. The distinguishing feature of this warning is the early onset of the red. Although this warning alternative has the advantage of a low installation cost relative to the other warnings, it is possible that the early red onset would not be compelling enough. Also, because the red is delayed 0.5 s relative to the other warnings, it may delay braking by up to 0.5 s and thus require even more aggressive braking than the other warnings.

Condition 2

The second warning condition was to immediately change the signal from green to red and to supplement the red signal lenses with additional signs and lights. Added red lenses (wig-wag lights) were situated on both sides of the normal red lens alternated at 1 Hz. This was combined with a variable message sign that was scaled to be 1 m (4 ft) on a side and was positioned between two signal heads, which were aligned with the approach lanes. When this otherwise black sign was activated, a large red hexagonal stop symbol was presented, and two expanding white spots simulated strobe lights in the lower corners of the sign. The spots alternated at 2 Hz. Figure 1 depicts a driver' view of this warning.

This is a screen capture from the driving simulation. It shows a traffic signal mast arm with a square sign placed equidistant between two traffic signal head assemblies. The traffic signal head assemblies have five lenses arranged in a T. Three red lenses are across the top of the T, and amber and green lenses, respectively, are below the center red lens. In the figure, only the center and left lenses are illuminated. The sign between the two signal head assemblies has a red octagon in the middle and a white strobe light illuminated in the lower right corner of the sign.

Figure 1. Illustration. Supplemental visual warning displays from the driving simulation.

Condition 3

The third warning condition included all the features from Condition 2, plus simulated intelligent rumble strips and simulated red light-emitting diodes (LEDs) along the lane markings. Intelligent rumble strips are devices that could be embedded in the roadway and would be detectable only when triggered by a collision warning system.(4) Intelligent rumble strips would produce the same auditory and tactile stimulation as the conventional rumble strips that are used as lane deviation and speed advisory warnings. Intelligent rumble strips were simulated by activating a 1.9-cm (0.75-inch) vertical heave of the simulator' motion base at 4 Hz for 1 s. The vertical heave resulted in both haptic and auditory stimulation that closely approximated the sensations generated by a series of four speed advisory rumble strips. Because of a programming error, the intelligent rumble strips were triggered when the participant vehicle was 61 m (200 ft) from the intersection stop line regardless of the trigger distance for the visual warnings. The simulated in-road LEDs were placed every 6 m (20 ft) along both the left- and right-lane markings of the through lane and every 60 cm (24 inches) across the intersection stop line. The simulated LEDs, are shown in figure 2.

This is a screen capture from the driving simulation. It shows an intersection with the Condition 3 warnings activated. On the far side of the intersection, the mast arm that is shown in figure 1 can be seen. On the near side of the intersection are four red rectangles that were intended to look like illuminated red LEDs. Two of these rectangles are on the right roadway edgeline about 8 and 16 m (27 and 52 ft) from the stop line. Another two of these rectangles are along the left edgeline or the right lane, also at 8 and 16 m (27 and 52 ft) from the stop line. Eight red rectangles, also simulating LEDs, are equally spaced along the far side of the stop line of the right lane.

Figure 2. Illustration. The simulated intersection with simulated in-road LEDs activated.

Trigger Distances

In addition to four signal conditions, two trigger distances were used for initiating the signal conditions: 66 m (215 ft) and 55 m (180 ft). Table 1 shows the two trigger distances and their metric equivalents. When the study began, the trigger distance for activating the warning and control conditions was 66 m (215 ft). Selection of this distance rested on an assumed perception-reaction time of 1 s and an instructed travel speed of 72 km/h (45 mi/h). Under these assumptions, a complete stop at the stop line in response to a warning would require 0.65 g of constant deceleration. It seemed reasonable to assume that no participant would attempt to stop for the amber signal given that deceleration requirement. However, the assumption proved to be wrong. Of the first 11 participants in the control (4-s amber) condition, 4 participants came to a complete stop before crossing the stop line. They were able to do this because perception-reaction times were less than 1 s and because decelerations peaked at 0.9 g (the maximum allowed by the vehicle dynamics model). Because such a high proportion of control condition participants were stopping, the trigger distance was reduced from 66 m (215 ft) to 55 m (180 ft). Assuming a 1-s response time, the 55-m (180-ft) trigger distance would require 0.85 g of constant deceleration. The warning trigger distance was 66 m (215 ft) for the first
52 participants and 55 m (180 ft) for the final 162 participants.

Table 1. Trigger distances and metric equivalents.
U.S. customary (ft) Metric (m)
180 ft 55 m
215 ft 66 m

1 ft = 0.305 m

Participants

Participants were licensed drivers recruited from the Washington, DC, metropolitan area. Useable data were obtained from 202 of 214 participants. Twelve participants (5.6 percent) failed to complete the experiment because of simulator sickness symptoms. There were two age groups (younger, 18 to 65 years of age; and older, 66 to 87 years of age). Age and gender were approximately balanced across the four warning conditions.

Protocol

Participants reviewed and signed an informed consent document upon arrival at the research center. They were then asked a series of questions to screen for impairment by illness, drugs, or alcohol. Vision was tested to ensure that they met or exceeded a minimum visual acuity of 20/40 in at least one eye. Next, participants were escorted to the driving simulator facility where they were asked to perform the Sharpened Romberg postural stability test and to complete the Simulator Sickness Questionnaire (SSQ).(9,10) The Sharpened Romberg and SSQ were completed before the participant entered the vehicle to provide baselines against which post-simulation health status could be assessed.
Following completion of these tests, participants were seated alone in the Federal Highway Administration (FHWA) Highway Driving Simulator. A researcher read the following instructions aloud:

The Federal Highway Administration provides guidelines and recommendations to the states regarding federally funded highway design and operations. One aspect of these guidelines has to do with the phasing of traffic signals. The purpose of this experiment is to evaluate various phasing patterns.

You are asked to drive along a two-lane rural road that has a traffic signal every half mile. Drive as you normally would while maintaining 45 mph, which is the posted speed limit. Obey all traffic laws and observe all traffic signals. When you need to slow down or stop for a traffic signal, accelerate back to 45 mph as soon as you get a green light. Except when you are responding to a traffic signal, if you exceed 50 mph, or slow below 40 mph, the experimenter will remind you that you should be maintaining 45 mph by saying "Watch your speed."

Pay attention to how the signals change. At the conclusion of your drive, which should take about 25 minutes, we will ask you questions about the signals. Thank you, and drive safely.

Simulated Roadway

The simulated roadway was a contiguous series of 1.3-km (0.8-mi) segments of a straight and level two-lane rural highway. In the middle of each segment, the same intersection recurred. At this intersection, the road widened to include a dedicated left-turn lane. Although the same roadway segment recurred 33 times, the roadside scenery varied slightly from segment to segment. Bridges, guardrails, trees, and fences close to the roadway remained the same on each segment, but widely spaced buildings (e.g., churches and houses) varied between segments. Except for the absence of vertical curvature in the simulation, the roadway was modeled on US 29 and the intersection of US 29 with State Route 234 in Manassas, VA.

Experimental Design

The research design was entirely between groups with only one observation per participant. Statistical power was achieved by the use of a relatively large participant sample size rather than by repeated measurement of the same participants. The between groups design was necessitated by the assumptions underlying the study. First, it was assumed that red-light violator warnings would be rare and that any individual might experience such a warning only once or twice over a driving career. If true, an individual' response to repeated warnings would be of little or no interest. Second, it was assumed that, as with most new traffic control devices, currently licensed drivers would receive little or no information about an infrastructure-based warning device. Given these assumptions, it was deemed imperative that any red-light violator warning be intuitive and quickly comprehensible on first exposure and without the requirement of previous training or experience.

As stated in the introduction, traffic signals previous to the final (warning) intersection were timed to build an expectation that the final signal would be functioning normally and would likely be green. Table 2 shows the signal functioning intended to build this expectation. Assuming a 1-s brake response time to the onset of an amber at any intersection before the 33rd, no decision to immediately brake would have required a deceleration of more than 1.8 m/s2 (5.9 ft/s2), or 0.18 g. Because deceleration rates and braking latency were under participants' control, the number of braking, stopping, and slowing events varied among participants. Based on the distance from the signal at amber onset and on the duration of the red, the table indicates the signals for which most participants came to a complete stop (one asterisk), and the signals for which no accelerator release or brake response was required to avoid braking at more than 3 m/s2 (10 ft/s2), or 0.31 g (two asterisks).

At the first two intersections, the signals remained green at all times. At all other intersections, there was an amber-phase onset. However, the amber and red phases occurred when the participant was far enough from the intersection that slowing or stopping was only required at seven intersections, which are indicated by a single asterisk in table 2. At the 33rd intersection, a 4-s amber was triggered at 531 m (1,741 ft). The red phase that followed was 14 s. The light then turned green. Assuming the participant maintained 72 km/h (45 mi/h), the vehicle would have been 169 m (553 ft) from the stop line when the light turned green. Given that up to that point, the signals had changed only once when the vehicle was closer than 169 m (553 ft) and that (at the instructed speed) the vehicle was only 8 s from the stop line at green onset, it was assumed that the participant would not be anticipating another phase change at the 33rd intersection.

Table 2. Signal change schedule for the first 32 intersections in the simulation.
Intersection Distance from intersection at onset of amber Duration of red
1 Always Green 0
2 Always Green 0
3 764* 11
4 967 11
5 1,043 11
6 1,075 11
7 1,092 11
8 1,102 11
9 1,102 11
10 433* 15
11 639* 15
12 1,036 11
13 1,049 11
14 1,075 11
15 986 11
16 1,042 11
17 770* 12
18 1,127 12
19 1,082 12
20 1,056 12
21 1,239 18
22 1,423* 18
23 961* 18
24 1,397 18
25 1,043* 18
26 1,269 10
27 1,334** 10
28 1,400** 10
29 1,469** 10
30 1,538** 10
31 1,603** 10
32 1,672** 10
33*** 1,741** 14

1 ft = 0.305 m
* Most participants stopped, others slowed.
** Could have proceeded without slowing.
*** Warning triggered after change back to green.

Independent Variables

Four between group variables were tested. Signal condition had four categories, and the other variables each had two categories:

  • Signal condition:
    • Control—4-s amber.
    • Condition 1—0.5-s amber.
    • Condition 2—wig-wag lights, stop symbol on variable message sign, and simulated strobes.
    • Condition 3—Condition 2 plus in-pavement LEDs and intelligent rumble strips.
  • Trigger distance:
    • 55 m (180 ft).
    • 66 m (215 ft).
  • Age groups:
    • Under 65 years of age.
    • 65 years of age or older.
  • Gender:
    • Male.
    • Female.

Dependent Measures

The dependent measures in this study were as follows:

  • Delay in reaching stop line.
  • Initial speed.
  • Accelerator release time.
  • Brake response time.
  • Maximum deceleration.

Table 3 summarizes the experimental design and shows the number of participants within each cell.

Table 3. Number of participants in experiment 1 as a function of experimental conditions (trigger distance and signal condition) and subject variables (gender and age group).
  Females > 65 Males > 65 Females < 66 Males < 66
180 ft Control 10 9 11 7
180 ft Condition 1 8 10 10 11
180 ft Condition 2 8 12 9 10
180 ft Condition 3 9 8 8 10
215 ft Control 2 1 3 5
215 ft Condition 1 2 1 5 4
215 ft Condition 2 2 2 5 4
215 ft Condition 3 2 4 6 4

1 ft = 0.305 m

Delay in Reaching Stop Line

Because the purpose of the warnings is to prevent drivers from being in the intersection when a red-light violator is crossing their path, it is critical that the warning delay their arrival to the intersection until the threat has passed. To evaluate warning effectiveness, a violator was assumed to be traveling at the same speed as the participant and to have been detected at the warning trigger distance (i.e., either 66 m (215 ft) or 55 m (180 ft)). Thus, if both vehicles proceeded at the same speed they were traveling when the violator was detected, then they would collide in the intersection. To allow some leeway for variation from the assumed scenario, a minimum of 1-s arrival delay of the potential victim (participant driver) was deemed to represent a success. If the violator continued at 72.5 km/h (45 mi/h), a 1-s delay in arrival of the participant would result in the participant passing 20 m (66 ft) behind the violator. The delay in arrival was calculated from each participant's actual speed, rather than the instructed speed. The amount of delay in seconds was then converted to a binary variable: 0 if less than 1 s, 1 if equal to or greater than 1 s.

Initial Speed

Although the delay in reaching stop line variable was calculated based on initial speed, the decision to delay or continue at the current speed could be influenced by the driver' speed when the warning is initiated. Because the warning was triggered based on distance, not time to intersection, lower speeds would allow lower deceleration rates, longer response times, or both. Therefore, initial speed was analyzed for possible influences on the other results.

Accelerator Release Time

Accelerator release time was measured because it provides the first available indication that the warning stimulus has been detected. Accelerator release time is sometimes referred to as perception time.(10)

Brake Response Time

Brake response time is the most frequently used measure of driver latency to react to the onset of amber signals. In the traffic engineering literature, brake response time is often referred to as perception-reaction time.(12) In field studies, brake response time is often measured from the onset of an amber signal to the onset of the vehicle brake lights (e.g., Wortman and Matthias, 1983; and Gates et al., 2007).(5) However, in this study, the beginning of brake pedal deflection is used as the terminator for brake response time.

Maximum Deceleration

Maximum deceleration was measured to suggest the intensity of the braking that followed the brake responses to the signal conditions. It was the largest frame-to-frame deceleration that was recorded between the onset of the amber/warning signal and crossing the stop line. It was only recorded for participants for whom a brake response time was recorded.

Results—Driving Simulation

Delay in Reaching Stop Line

The primary measure of a collision warning' effectiveness is whether or not a collision is avoided. The hypothetical threat for which the signal conditions were triggered was a red-light violator who approached the intersection on the cross street. A 1 s or greater delay of the participant in reaching the intersection, based on the participant' speed when the warning was initiated, was counted as an avoided collision.

A logit model was used to analyze the delay results. A logit analysis was used because the independent variables were classification variables and the dependent variable (delay) was binary. Although logit analysis is similar to the more familiar analysis of variance in that it includes both main effects and interactions, it differs in the way statistical significance is evaluated. Rather than providing statistical significance for each effect, the model as a whole is evaluated, with effects dropped from the model until the model no longer accounts for the observed variability. In a logit analysis, the best fitting model is the most parsimonious model for which the goodness-of-fit does not differ significantly from the full model.(13,14) That is, variables and interactions are dropped from the model until a model with the fewest variables that still enables reconstruction of the obtained pattern of results is identified. The dependent measure was the 1-s delay (yes or no). The independent variables in the full (saturated) logit model were age, gender, signal condition, trigger distance, and all interactions of these variables. The final model included signal condition, age, trigger distance, and the interaction of trigger distance with signal condition. Table 4 shows the results as a function of the variables in the best fitting model. Older drivers were more likely to be delayed regardless of trigger distance or warning condition.

Table 4. The percentage of participants delayed by a least 1 s in reaching the stop line shown as a function of age group, experimental condition, and warning trigger distance.

Age Group
Trigger Distance Signal Condition Delayed
Under 65 180 ft Control 17%
Under 65 180 ft Condition 1 17%
Under 65 180 ft Condition 2 48%
Under 65 180 ft Condition 3 47%
       
Under 65 215 ft Control 38%
Under 65 215 ft Condition 1 60%
Under 65 215 ft Condition 2 78%
Under 65 215 ft Condition 3 89%
       
65 or Over 180 ft Control 37%
65 or Over 180 ft Condition 1 71%
65 or Over 180 ft Condition 2 61%
65 or Over 180 ft Condition 3 70%
       
65 or Over 215 ft Control 67%
65 or Over 215 ft Condition 1 67%
65 or Over 215 ft Condition 2 100%
65 or Over 215 ft Condition 3 100%

1 ft = 0.305 m

However, it is wrong to infer the greater effectiveness of the warning with older adults is directly attributable to age. As reported below, older adults were, on average, traveling slower when the warnings came on. Initial speed can also predict delay outcome. Indeed, when initial speed is entered as a covariate in the logit model, age and the interaction of trigger distance with signal condition can be dropped from the model with no significant loss in prediction. Table 5 shows the percent of participants delayed as a function of the two variables that remained significant after accounting for initial speed—signal condition and trigger distance.

The warning treatments were more effective at 66 m (215 ft) than at 55 m (180 ft). The greater the conspicuity of the warning, the greater the proportion of participants who delayed.

Table 5. Percentage of participants delayed by at least 1 s in reaching the stop line shown as a function of experimental condition and warning trigger distance.

Trigger Distance
Experimental Condition Delayed
180 ft Control 27%
180 ft Condition 1 43%
180 ft Condition 2 54%
180 ft Condition 3 59%
180 ft All Conditions 46%
215 ft Control 45%
215 ft Condition 1 63%
215 ft Condition 2 83%
215 ft Condition 3 92%
215 ft All Conditions 71%

1 ft = 0.305 m

Table 6 shows the percentage of participants delayed without regard to trigger distance.

Table 6. Percentage of participants that were delayed in reaching the stop line by 1 s or more shown as a function of condition.
Signal Condition Delayed
Control 31%
Condition 1 49%
Condition 2 61%
Condition 3 67%

Initial Speed

Among the younger adults, the instruction to maintain 72 km/h (45 mi/h) was successful. At the beginning of the final phase change event (i.e., onset of the warning or amber signal), the mean speed of the younger adult groups was 71.5 km/h (44.4 mi/h) with a standard deviation of 6.6 km/h (4.1 mi/h). The older adults tended to drive below the instructed speed. They had a mean speed at the beginning of the final phase change event of 66.8 km/h (41.5 mi/h) with a standard deviation of 7.4 km/h (4.6 mi/h). Initial speed was not significantly related to any of the other independent variables (all p > 0.17).

Accelerator Release Response Time

Of the 202 participants, 156 responded with accelerator releases between the onset of the warning/amber phase and arrival at the stop line. Of these 156 accelerator releases, 4 response times were deemed to be outliers and excluded from further analyses. There were no outliers with extreme short responses. Long response outliers were identified as responses that were more than 2 s. Those responses were more than 1.5 times the interquartile range from the upper edge of that range. The interquartile range is defined as the distance between the 25th-percentile response time and the 75th-percentile response time. Of the four response times deemed outliers, one came from an older adult and three came from younger adults. Assuming a 72 km/h (45 mi/h) approach speed, 2 s after the onset of the warning participants would have been 25 m (83 ft) or 15 m (48 ft) from the stop line, depending on whether the trigger distance was 66 m (215 ft) or 55 m (180 ft), respectively.

A full factorial analysis of variance was conducted on the 152 remaining accelerator release times. Signal condition, age group, gender, and trigger distance were the independent variables. The significant main effects are represented in figure 3 where it can be seen that responses at the 66-m (215-ft) trigger distance were faster than at the 55-m (180-ft) trigger distance,
F (1, 120) = 3.7, p < 0.05, and that responses to the 0.5-s amber were longer than to the other conditions, F (3, 120) = 12.7, p < 0.01.

This line graph shows the trigger distances, 55 and 66 m (180 and 215 ft), on the abscissa and the mean throttle response times (in seconds) on the ordinate. The grouping factor is warning condition. Overall, regardless of the condition, response times were shorter when the warning occurred at 66 m (215 ft), with the smallest difference between the trigger distances being 0.1 s in Condition 3 and the largest difference being 0.3 s in Condition 2. Condition 2 stands out with the longest response times, which were about 0.17 s longer than the closest response times. The response times at 55 m (180 ft) were 0.88, 1.06, 0.86, and 0.82 s for the control and Conditions 1 through 3, respectively. The response times at 66 m (215 ft) were 0.68, 0.84, 0.56, and 0.72 s for the control and Conditions 1 through 3, respectively.

1 ft = 0.305 m

Figure 3. Line graph. Mean accelerator release times as a function of experimental condition and distance at which the phase change was triggered.

Although the warning condition effect was significant, it was solely attributable to the male drivers. As seen in figure 4, gender and experimental condition interacted, F (3, 120) = 3.9,p < 0.05, such that females showed almost no difference in response time across the four warning conditions.

This bar graph shows the trigger distances, 55 m and 66 m (180 and 215 ft), on the abscissa and the mean throttle response times (in seconds) on the ordinate. The grouping factor is gender. Among females, the mean throttle response time was nearly invariant. Across conditions, females had response times that ranged from 0.76 s to 0.84 s, and the 95-percent confidence limits averaged ± 0.08 s. However, the males' response times in Condition 1 were 0.3 s longer than in the control. Response times in Conditions 2 and 3 were significantly shorter than in the control and were at least 0.1 s less than the control with confidence limits that were the same as for the females (± 0.08 s).

Figure 4. Bar graph. Gender by condition interaction in accelerator release time.

The above accelerator release times were not conditional on whether the participant subsequently braked or came to a stop. When only the accelerator releases of participants who subsequently were delayed by at least 1 s are considered, then the results are quite different. In the case of these 86 participants, the only significant factor was age, F (1, 56) = 7.1, p < 0.05, where drivers in the older group began their accelerator release an average of 0.16 s later than drivers in the younger group.

Brake Application Response Time

Initial analyses of the brake response times indicated a number of outliers, as defined in the preceding section. Exclusion of participants with brake response times greater than 2.2 s or less than 0.3 s reduced the sample of brake response times from 167 to 159. A full factorial analysis of variance that included condition, age group, trigger distance, and gender as variables indicated there were no statistically reliable effects. Analysis of variance that included only signal condition as a variable also failed to reveal any effects to be significant at the 0.05 level. Overall, the mean brake response time was 1.21 s with a standard error of 0.02 s.

Maximum Deceleration

Participants who were delayed in reaching the stop line by at least 1 s had a mean for maximum deceleration of 0.78 g (standard error = 0.01). Figure 5 and figure 6 show plots of the deceleration and braking behavior of two participants from Condition 3 who responded by stopping following the warning triggered at 55 m (180 ft) from the stop line. The top panel shows data from a 19-year-old participant who appeared to hesitate before releasing the accelerator and hesitated again before exerting near maximum braking force. The lower panel shows data from a 66-year-old participant who showed less hesitation in exerting maximum braking and then coasted to the stop line.

Time (s)

Text Box: g or Proportion Brake/AcceleratorText Box: Speed (mi/h) and Distance (ft)This figure's abscissa shows time from 0.0 s after the warning was triggered to 4.5 s after the warning. There are two ordinates. The left ordinate shows speed in miles per hour and distance in feet from 0 to 200 (a conversion equation to metric units is provided). The right ordinate shows g and proportion of brake pedal or accelerator pedal defection from 0 to 0.90. Data for five variables are plotted: speed, distance to stop bar, accelerator pedal deflection, brake pedal deflection, and deceleration force. The figure shows data for a participant that began accelerator pedal release at about 1 s after the warning, began brake pedal pressing at about 1.3 s after the warning, and gradually increased deceleration force to about 0.79 g 2.8 s after the warning. Hard braking, greater than 0.5 g, continued until the speed came to 0 mi/h as the car reached the stop line.

1 ft = 0.305 m
1 mi/h = 1.61 km/h

Figure 5. Line graph. Example 1 of data from Condition 3 participants who responded by stopping.

Time (s)

Text Box: g or Proportion Brake/AcceleratorText Box: Speed (mi/h) and Distance (ft)This figure's abscissa shows time from 0.0 s after the warning was triggered to 4.5 s after the warning. There are two ordinates. The left ordinate shows speed in miles per hour and distance in feet from 0 to 200 (a conversion equation to metric units is provided). The right ordinate shows g and proportion of brake pedal or accelerator pedal defection from 0 to 0.90. Data for five variables are plotted: speed, distance to stop bar, accelerator pedal deflection, brake pedal deflection, and deceleration force. The figure shows a different response pattern from figure 5 in that this driver began releasing the accelerator about 0.8 s after the warning, was on the brake within 1 s, and reached a maximum deceleration of about 0.75 g only 1.5 s after the onset of the warning. The participant came to a complete stop when still 6 m (20 ft) upstream of the stop bar, about 4 s after the onset of the warning. After coming to a complete stop, the participant accelerated gently to reduce the distance to the stop bar.

1 ft = 0.305 m
1 mi/h = 1.61 km/h

Figure 6. Line graph. Example 2 of data from Condition 3 participants who responded by stopping.

Studies that measure deceleration from film or video usually report the deceleration rate as the change in velocity from when the brakes lights illuminate until the vehicle stops. This is a different measure than the maximum deceleration, which is reported in the previous figures. For comparison with studies that report deceleration rate, a comparable measure was computed for the Condition 3 participants who were delayed by 1 s or more. This measure covered the interval between when the brake response began until the participant either: (1) crossed the stop line,
(2) came to a complete stop, or (3) ceased decelerating. The latter was used when a participant braked so hard that he or she was approaching a stop well short of the stop line and began accelerating or coasting to close that distance, as was the case for the participant whose data are plotted in. The 15th- and 85th-percentile deceleration rates were 0.50 g (5.73 m/s (18.79 ft/s)) and 0.63 g (5.78 m/s (18.94 ft/s)). Gates et al. reported 50th-percentile deceleration rates for first to stop vehicles of 0.31 g (3.05 m/s (10 ft/s)).(5) Wortman and Matthias report 50th-percentile decelerations of about 0.45 g (3.5 m/s (11.6 ft/s)) for the first car to stop at signalized intersections in Phoenix and Tucson.(15,16)

Discussion—Driving Simulation

The present finding that 92 percent (Condition 3, trigger distance 66 m (215 ft)) of potential victims could be delayed (using the Ferlis assumption that 80 percent of potential violators might be stopped by a warning) suggests the estimated number of collisions resulting from red light violations that could be prevented could rise to 99 percent. Of course a 90-percent success rate may be optimistic. However, if only 59 percent of potential victims responded, as was the case for Condition 3 with a 55-m (180-ft) trigger distance, and only 50 percent of violators could be stopped, then the combined crash reduction due to victim and violator responses would still be a substantial 80 percent.

Test track (Experiment 2)

Experiment 2 was conducted to validate the following findings from the driving simulation:

  • About 54 percent of drivers might respond to a conspicuous infrastructure-based warning.
  • A high percentage of drivers (27 percent in the driving simulation) who are traveling at about 72 km/h (45 mi/h) would stop for an amber signal that comes on when they are 55 m (180 ft) from an intersection.
  • The mean peak deceleration of 0.78 g is comparable to the decelerations that might be seen in a real-world warning situation.

Methods—Test track

The test track experiment was conducted on the Virginia Smart Road at the Virginia Tech Transportation Institute. A late-model passenger car was used, and the road was closed to traffic. The violator warning closely resembled the mast arm warning stimulus used in the driving simulation. It incorporated an immediate change from green to red, alternating flashing red beacons on either side of the solid red lens, a changeable message sign with stop symbol, and strobe lights that alternated at 2 Hz. The test track warning did not include intelligent rumble strips because that technology does not yet exist, and it did not include in-roadway LEDs because of cost.

Conditions Tested

Only one warning condition was tested, and this condition was similar to Condition 2 in the driving simulation. The mast arm warning used on the test track is shown in figure 7. It varied from the simulator mast arm in that the solid red stop symbol was replaced with an octagonal red outline that enclosed the word "STOP," and brighter strobes were suspended below the sign. The strobe lights within the changeable message sign that can be seen in figure 7 were disconnected and did not flash.

This photo shows the mast arm configuration for the field test. It shows two five-lens signal head assemblies mounted on either side of a square sign. The five lenses are arranged in a T with three red lenses across the top and amber and green lenses down the shaft of the T. In the photo, the warning is active, so two red lenses are illuminated on each of the two five-lens assemblies. The sign has a black background. Two concentric octagons are outlined in red LEDs. The word STOP is contained within the concentric octagons, which also consists of pairs of red LEDs. Below the sign is a horizontal metal bar that has two strobe lights attached at each end of the bar. One of the strobe lights is illuminated.


Figure 7. Photo. Warning configuration on the test track.

Trigger Distances

Two warning trigger distances were used for the driving simulation, one at a 32-m (105-ft) distance and the other at a 55-m (180-ft) distance. The 32-m (105-ft) distance was selected as a distance at which stopping responses would be improbable but still close enough that drivers might attempt to brake in response. Table 7 shows the time to the stop line from 32 m (105 ft) and 55 m (180 ft), given a 72 km/h (45 mi/h) approach speed. It also shows the constant deceleration that would be required given a 1-s delay for driver reaction time and brake system response lag.

Table 7. Time to stop line when warning is triggered, and required constant deceleration to stop at stop line assuming a 1 s delay before the deceleration begins.
Distance to Stop line

Metric Distance

Time to Stop line

Required Deceleration (g)

105 ft

32 m

1.6 s

1.6

180 ft

55 m

2.7 s

0.6

1 ft = 0.305 m

Participants

Participants were licensed drivers recruited from the Virginia Tech participant database, by word-of-mouth, and through advertisements in a local Blacksburg, VA, newspaper. Participants were randomly assigned to warning distance groups with the constraint that roughly equal numbers of younger and older males and females were assigned to each group. The younger group consisted of 32 persons, with a mean age of 40 years and a range of 20 to 64 years. The older group had a mean age of 71 years, with a range of 65 to 82 years. Table 8 shows the demographic distribution of the participants among conditions.

Table 8. Age and gender distribution of participants in the two warning distance conditions.
  20–64-year-old males 20–64-year-old females 65–85-year-old males 65–85-year-old females Total
Warning at 105 ft 8 7 7 8 30
Warning at 180 ft 8 9 7 6 30

Total

16 16 14 14 60

1 ft = 0.305 m

Protocol

Participants were told that the experiment was an evaluation of signal timing. They were not told that an emergency warning would be presented or that the research concerned red light running. They were instructed to maintain a speed of 72 km/h (45 mi/h) except when turning around or responding to the traffic signal. They traveled through the intersection 12 times in each direction before the warning was issued on the 25th approach.

The instructions to participants were as follows:

In this study, we ask that you drive as you normally would and obey all traffic laws. Assume that the speed limit is 45 mph, and try to maintain that speed when not constrained by curves or traffic signals. The road you will be driving on is closed to other traffic. However, you should remain alert to hazards that might include other vehicles, animals, or debris on the roadway. The experimenter' assistant will prompt you to "watch your speed" if, for no apparent reason, you exceed 50 mph or drop below 40 mph. That prompt is a reminder that you should maintain 45 mph when cruising.

The road on which you will drive has one traffic signal. Respond to that signal as you normally respond to traffic signals. We are interested in how the timing of signal changes affects driver response, so it is important that you respond the way you would on open roadways where other vehicles might be present.

There is a turnaround at each end of the closed road. Continue to use these turnarounds until the experimenter's assistant says that the experiment is over. However, should you feel the need to take a break, particularly if you become drowsy or fatigued, inform the assistant that you need to take a break. You may request a break at any time, but it is best if you request the break immediately after you have passed the traffic signal (not while coming up to the signal).
Your safety is of highest concern. Above all drive carefully and do not take risks. You may discontinue participation at any time and without penalty should you feel that you are asked to do anything that is unsafe or improper.

Do you have any questions?

In the test track experiment, the amber was triggered at distances relatively near to the stop line on 20 of the first 24 approaches to the traffic signal. Each approach was considered to be a trial. The 4-s amber was triggered four times at each of five distances, where distance was measured between the front of the vehicle and the stop line. On only four test track trials did the signal remain green. Table 9 shows the five amber trigger distances and their metric equivalents. The order of trials with the various approach distance triggers was randomized separately for each participant, except the emergency warning, which was always on the 25th trail. Each trial condition except the warning was presented twice on upgrade legs and twice on downgrade legs. The emergency warning trial always occurred on the upgrade.

Table 9. Amber signal trigger distances and their metric equivalents.
U.S. Customary Metric
105 ft 32 m
180 ft 55 m
215 ft 66 m
290 ft 88 m
365 ft 111 m

1 ft = 0.305 m

On the 25th trial, half the participants were warned at 55 m (180 ft) from the intersection, and half were warned at 32 m (105 ft). One concern for use of infrastructure to warn potential victims of red-light violators is that drivers who are close enough to the intersection to pass through before the violator arrives might be slowed by the warning and then become a victim because of their response to the warning. This concern has led to the suggestion that the warning be designed to be visible only to the drivers who need to be warned.(4) The 32-m (105-ft) warning distance was included to test whether or not it might be so close that no driver would respond. Table 7 shows the time to stop line from 32 m (105 ft) and 55 m (180 ft) given a 73-km/h (45‑mi/h) approach speed. It also shows the constant deceleration that would be required given a 1-s delay for driver reaction time and brake system response lag. Under the test conditions on the Smart Road, the test vehicle' antilock braking system limited deceleration to about 0.89 g. With hard deceleration, a stop is possible in response to a warning from 55 m (180 ft), but stopping for a warning from 32 m (105 ft) would seem improbable. If drivers in this study brake hard for the warning that is triggered at 32 m (105 ft), this would indicate that the design of a violator warning device would need to consider which drivers can see the warning. If drivers do not respond to the warning that is triggered at 32 m (105 ft), this would imply that shielding the warning from those for whom it is not intended might not be critical.

Participants were tested individually under good driving conditions (daylight, dry pavement, etc.). Before the first trial, each participant drove down the Smart Road to its end (2.7 km (1.7 mi)). During that drive, participants were asked to accelerate and brake several times to familiarize themselves with the vehicle' handling and braking characteristics. Before the first experimental trial began, participants were required to brake "as hard as you can" from a speed of 72 km/h (45 mi/h). This request was repeated until the participant decelerated to a minimum of 0.45 g. Two experimenters were present in the car with participant drivers. The experimenter in the front passenger seat served as a safety observer and was also tasked with providing instructions and answering questions from the participant. The other experimenter sat behind the participant and monitored the on-board computer. The on-board computer was used to send trial information to the traffic controller and to monitor sensor outputs.

Test Road

As configured for this test, the Virginia Smart Road had two lanes (one in each direction) and one signalized intersection. Participants drove a 1.6-km (1-mi) approach to the intersection followed by a 0.5-km (0.3-mi) leg to a high-speed turnaround and another 0.5-km (0.3-mi) approach. The road had a 3-percent grade. Because participants turned around at the end of each run, half the trials were on a 3-percent upgrade and half were on a 3-percent downgrade. The Virginia Smart Road intersection is pictured in figure 8.

This picture shows a car stopped at the stop line of the intersection on the Virginia Smart Road. The mast arm warning that is shown in figure 6 can be seen at the far side of the intersection. In the photo, the road curves gently to the left at the intersection.

Figure 8. Photo. Virginia Smart Road intersection and the vehicle used for testing.

Test Vehicle

A 2004 Chevrolet Impala equipped with a differential global position system receiver, a longitudinal accelerometer, and sensors for accelerator position and brake pressure was used for the test. The vehicle data stream was synchronized with changes in the traffic signal controller via a wireless 802.11 communications link. Accelerator and brake position were recorded at 100 Hz, and all other sensor and vehicle data were recorded at 10 Hz.

Results—Test track

Dependent variables were the same as in the simulator experiment: initial speed, delay, accelerator release time, and brake response time.

Initial Speed

Amber Signal

Unlike the simulation study, initial speed did not vary between age groups. Overall, the mean initial speed was 73.7 km/h (45.8 mi/h). Females approached the intersections more slowly than males, F (1, 56) = 5.0, p < 0.05. The mean approach speed of females was 73.4 km/h (45.6 mi/h), and for males it was 74.5 km/h (46.3 mi/h). This small, but statistically reliable gender difference in mean approach speed, was the result of females slowing more as they got closer to the intersections, whereas the males generally maintained the same (higher) speed until the signal change was triggered. The interaction of gender with the linear trend in speed was significant, F (1, 56) = 7.5, p < 0.01. The gender by linear trend interaction can be seen in figure 9. Error bars in figure 9 represent two standard errors of the mean. On average, males accelerated slightly between 55 m (180 ft) and 32 m (105 ft); their speed at 32 m (105 ft) was significantly faster than at 55 m (180 ft), F ( 1, 29) = 5.4, p < 0.05.

This figures shows the speed of males and females as a function of distance from the intersection when the traffic signal turned amber. The abscissa shows distance from the stop line in ft with data points at 32, 55, 66, 88, and 111 m (105, 180, 215, 290, and 365 ft) (a conversion equation is provided below the figure to determine the metric equivalent). The ordinate shows mean speed in miles per hour from 68 to 77 km/h (42 to 48 mi/h) (a conversion equation is provided below the figure to determine the metric equivalent). The grouping variable is gender. The figure shows a steady decline in the mean speed of females from just under 74 km/h (46 mi/h) when 111 m (365 ft) from the intersection to 72 km/h (45 mi/h) when 32 m (105 ft) from the stop line. In contrast, the males had a mean speed of just above 74 km/h (46 mi/h) when 111 m (365 ft) from the intersection and about the same mean speed at all points between 111 and 55 m (365 and 180 ft) from the stop line. The mean speed of males was about 75.2 km/h (46.7 mi/h) at 323 m (105 ft) from the stop line. The 95-percent confidence limits for the means are shown. The confidence limits are about ±0.4 km/h (± 0.25 mi/h) at 111 m (365 ft) from the stop line and gradually increase so that they are slightly greater than 0.8 km/h (0.5 mi/h) at 32 m (105 ft) from the stop line.

1 mi/h = 1.61 km/h
1 ft = 0.305 m

Figure 9. Line graph. Mean speed at the onset of the amber signals as a function of gender and amber trigger distance.

Warning Signal

Mean speed when the warning was triggered was 72.3 km/h (44.9 mi/h) and did not vary significantly as a function of age, gender, or warning onset distance. Although the same gender by trigger distance trend appeared in the warning response initial speeds (females' mean speed was less at 32 m (105 ft) than at 55 m (180 ft), whereas males' mean speed was the same at both distances), in this case the interaction was not statistically reliable (p = 0.09).

Delay with 4-s Amber

The driver responses to the amber signals on their first 24 passes through the intersection have been extensively reported elsewhere. (See references 6, 7, 8, and 17.) This report is not intended to repeat those reports but rather is focused on the proportion of drivers delayed by 1 s or more by the onset of the amber signals. However, because all drivers who were delayed by 1 s or more also came to a full stop, the likelihood of stopping is reported. This likelihood of stopping may be compared to the likelihood of delay in the control condition of the driving simulation.

No trends were identified in the likelihood of participants stopping as a function of the number of previous passes through the intersection. Participants stopped for 55 percent of the 4-s ambers that were triggered at 55 m (180 ft). Participants were more likely to stop for the 55-m (180-ft) amber onset in the test track experiment than in the simulation, 2 (1) = 21.4, p < 0.01. This finding supports the hypothesis that 55 m (180 ft) is within the dilemma zone when travel speed is 72 km/h (45 mi/h).

Table 10. The percent of trials in which participants stopped response to 4-s amber onset shown as a function of trigger distance.
Distance (ft) Percent of trials in which driver stopped
105 3%
180 55%
215 81%
290 95%
365 100%

1 ft = 0.305 m

Delay Attributable to Warning

When the warning was triggered at 55 m (180 ft), 27 of the 30 participants (90 percent) stopped and thus were delayed by more than 1 s. Of the three who did not stop, one was an older female (age 68), one a younger male (aged 20), and one an older male (aged 73). When the warning was triggered at 32 m (105 ft), 29 of the 30 participants (97 percent) did not stop and were not delayed by 1 s or more. The participant who stopped for the warning that was triggered at 32 m (105 ft) was a 72-year-old female. Of participants who stopped at either warning trigger distance, none went more than 5.2 m (17 ft) beyond the stop line. Figure 10 shows a comparison of the percentage of drivers who stopped for the amber warnings that were triggered at 32 m (105 ft) and 55 m (180 ft), with the percentage of drivers who stopped when the warning was triggered at these distances. As can be seen in the figure, there was no difference between responses to amber onset and warning onset at 32 m (105 ft), but the warning at 55 m (180 ft) was effective.

This bar graph shows the trigger distances, 32 m and 55 m (105 ft and 180 ft), on the abscissa and the percent of drivers who stopped on the ordinate. The grouping factor is amber and warning onsets. At 32 m (195 ft), the same percentage of drivers stopped for amber and warning onsets (approximately 4 percent). However, at 55 m (180 ft), the percentage of drivers who stopped for the warning onset was significantly higher (approximately 90 percent), showing that the warning onset was more effective at a greater distance; the percentage of drivers who stopped for the amber onset at this distance remained the same as at the shorter distance.

1 ft = 0.305 m

Figure 10. Bar graph. The percent of drivers in the test track experiment who stopped in response to amber and warning onsets at 32 m (105 ft) and 55 m (180 ft).

Based on the initial speed, none of the 29 drivers who received the warning at 32 m (105 ft) and did not stop were delayed more than 312 ms. Figure 11 shows that 85 percent of the delays of those who did not stop were less than 150 ms.

This figure shows the cumulative distribution of seconds of delay in reaching the stop line when the warning came on at 32 m (105 ft). Delay in milliseconds is shown on the abscissa and cumulative percent is on the ordinate. The plot shows that approximately 15 percent of drivers were delayed by 30 ms or less, 50 percent were delay by 100 ms or less, 85 percent were delayed by 160 ms or less, and 100 percent were delayed by 317 ms or less.

Figure 11. Line graph. Cumulative percent of driver delays in reaching the stop line when the warning was triggered at 32 m (105 ft) (n = 29).

Accelerator Release Response Time

Accelerator release response time was the interval between the onset of the amber signal and when the participant began decreasing accelerator position, with the constraint that the accelerator position continued to zero without reversal. If the accelerator was not depressed when the signal changed, which was a frequent occurrence on the downgrade, no response time was available. Only three accelerator releases were recorded following the onset of amber signals with the 32 m (105 ft) trigger distance with the mean equal to 318 ms; therefore, that distance is omitted from figure 12 where mean accelerator release times following amber signal onset are summarized. Error bars represent the one standard error of the mean. The means were computed without regard to participants, and the number of responses contributing to each mean varied between 17 and 30. Larger standard errors are the result of smaller samples. It can be seen in figure 12 that, at the shorter trigger distances, the accelerator release averaged about 350 ms. At longer distances on the upgrade, participants' response times were longer.

Distance from Stop Line (ft)

Text Box: Mean Accelerator Response Time (ms)TThis plot shows accelerator pedal release time as a function of the distance to the stop at which the warning was triggered and grade. Distance to the stop line is on the abscissa and accelerator response time is on the ordinate. Upgrade and downgrade are the factors. At 55 m and 66 m (180 ft and at 215 ft), mean upgrade and downgrade responses were nearly equal at about 355 ms. At 88 m (290 ft), as indicated by error bars for the standard errors of the mean, mean responses on the downgrade were significantly faster than on the upgrade or at any other distance. On the downgrade at 66 m (215 ft), mean response time was 303 ms. On the upgrade, mean response time was nearly 500 ms. At 111 m (365 ft), mean response on the downgrade was 391 ms, and mean response on the upgrade was 450 ms.

1 ft = 0.305 m

Figure 12. Line graph. Mean accelerator release times to amber onset are shown as a function of grade and trigger distance.

Brake Application Response Time

As for the driving simulation, brake application response time was the interval between the onset of the amber phase and the beginning of a brake application. Because not all participants braked at all distances and because participants may have been applying the brake when the amber began, the number of participants contributing to the brake response times shown in figure 13 varied from 3 at 32 m (105 ft) to 57 at 88 m (290 ft). The standard error of the mean is shown for downgrade response times. Standard errors on the upgrade were of similar magnitude, except for a larger error at the shortest onset distance. Response times tended to increase with distance.

This plot shows brake response time to the warning as a function of the distance at which the warning was triggered. Responses on the upgrade and downgrade are plotted as separate lines. Distance from the stop line in feet is on the abscissa. Mean brake response time in milliseconds is on the ordinate. On the downgrade, response times were 442, 529, 514, 624, and 730 ms when triggered at 32, 55, 66, 88, and 111 m (105, 180, 215, 290, and 365 ft), respectively. On the upgrade, brake response times were 477, 538, 561, 593, and 777 ms when triggered at 32, 55, 66, 88, and 111 ft (105, 180, 215, 290, and 365 ft), respectively.

1 ft = 0.305 m

Figure 13. Line graph. Mean brake response time as a function of amber trigger distance and grade.

Maximum Deceleration

The average peak deceleration for the 27 participants that stopped for the warning at 55 m (180 ) was 0.67 g. This is 0.1 g less than the maximum deceleration average for the simulation study.

Discussion—Test Track

The test track results confirmed the findings from the driving simulation that drivers would decelerate rapidly for a conspicuous and novel infrastructure based warning. The finding that 90 percent of test track drivers brought the vehicle to a complete stop within 4.6-m (15-ft) downstream of the stop line when the warning was triggered at 55-m (180-ft) upstream suggests that the hard braking observed in the driving simulation was not an artifact of the artificial simulation environment. The willingness of participants on the test track to exert near maximum braking demonstrates that the lack of physical deceleration forces in the simulation does not explain the hard braking observed in the simulation.

When the warning was triggered at 32 m (105 ft) from the stop line on the test track, 29 of 30 drivers did not stop or slow significantly. The one driver who did stop when warned at this distance was delayed by more than 1 s and thus would not have been put at risk of collision with a red-light violator who did not slow down. This finding suggests that it may not be necessary to mask warnings from the view of drivers that are too close to an intersection to respond. However, the possibility remains that there may be an intermediate zone where drivers are less consistent in their stop versus go behavior. Therefore, the need to mask the warning from drivers for whom the warning is not intended deserves further research.

Brake and accelerator release times on the test track were shorter than those observed in the simulator. The significance of this difference is discussed in the next section of this report.

General Discussion—Driving Simulation and Test track

The purpose of the simulation and test track studies was to evaluate the feasibility of warning potential victims of red-light violators. The results of the simulation study suggest that more than half of drivers would respond appropriately to an unexpected and unfamiliar warning if that warning were sufficiently conspicuous and distinct from a normal phase change. Although the simulator results are encouraging, many questions remain. Because the decelerations observed in the simulation were severe, one of those questions is whether or not drivers of actual vehicles on a real road would be willing to decelerate as severely to such a warning. To address the question of how drivers would behave on an actual roadway, one of the simulation conditions was replicated on a closed roadway. The results of that replication validate the simulation. Participants on the test track braked almost as severely as those in the simulation. The slightly greater 0.1-g deceleration that was observed in the simulation was probably attributable to the test track study participants' superior ability to modulate their approach to the stop line because of the better distance cues provided by the realworld environment. In the simulator, participants frequently stopped or approached zero speed far short of the stop line—as much as 15-m (50-ft) upstream of the stop line. Such behavior was not observed in the field where most participants modulated the deceleration to come to rest within ± 0.9 m (±3 ft) of the stop line.

Although 90 percent of the drivers stopped for the warning on the test track and only 64 percent stopped in the simulator (with warnings triggered at either 55 m (180) ft or 66 (215 ft)), the available evidence does not indicate which of these percentages is closer to what might be expected in an operational environment. However, there are some clues in the data. The accelerator and brake response time means from the test track experiment are close to those Olson and Sivak reported for "anticipated" stimuli.(12) This suggests that the events that led up to the emergency warning led participants to anticipate a stopping response. Before the warning, participants had driven through the intersection 24 times and had been presented with amber phase changes 20 times. Thus, although it is unlikely that participants anticipated the warning, they may well have anticipated an amber phase change, and many had stopped for amber onsets at the 55-m (180-ft) and 66-m (215-ft) trigger distances. This anticipation does not invalidate the test track findings, but it does raise the possibility that with fewer amber signals near the dilemma zone boundary, the test track findings might have been more similar to the "surprise" reaction times reported by Olson and Sivak.(10) The simulation accelerator and brake response times closely approximate those reported by Olson and Sivak in their "surprise" condition.

Conclusions and Recommendations for Further Research

The results of these experiments show that drivers are likely to respond appropriately by stopping or slowing for an infrastructure-based warning that they have not previously experienced. However, before recommending that such an infrastructure-based warning system be deployed, several other questions need to be answered. First among these is the effect of the presence of other vehicles. In both of these studies, other vehicles were absent, and now that there is evidence that drivers will respond to warnings in the absence of other vehicles, it is necessary to determine what drivers would do in a stream of vehicles approaching an intersection. In particular, the following questions should be considered:

  • Would drivers who know there are vehicles not too far behind them brake aggressively in response to a warning?
  • If a driver sees preceding vehicles not braking in response to a warning, is that driver less likely to brake?
  • What is the probability that warnings would precipitate rear-end collisions, and how severe are those collisions likely to be?

Although any rear-end collision that might result from a warning would likely be less severe than a side-impact collision with a red-light violator, further development of the warn-the-victim concept includes a need to examine and quantify the potential for rear-end collisions that may result from signal violation warnings. Research to address remaining questions concerning red-light violation warnings is currently being conducted by the FHWA.

References

  1. Najm, W.G., Smith, J.D., and Smith, D.L. Analysis of Crossing Path Crashes. Report No. DOT HS 809 423, U.S. Department of Transportation, Washington, DC, 2001.

  2. National Transportation Highway Safety Administration, Fatal Accident Reporting System (FARS). Accessed online: March 13, 2008. (http://www-fars.nhtsa.dot.gov)

  3. Zimmerman, K. and Bonneson, J.A. "Investigation of the Time-Into-Red for Red-Light-Related Crashes." Transportation Research Record, 1922, pp. 21–28, Washington, DC, 2005.

  4. Ferlis, R.A., Infrastructure-Based Collision Avoidance Systems. ITE Spring Conference and Exhibit, Palm Harbor, FL, 2002.

  5. Gates, T.F., Noyce, D.A., and Laracuente, L. "Analysis of Dilemma Zone Driver Behavior at Signalized Intersections." Transportation Research Board 86th Annual Meeting, Washington, DC, 2007.

  6. El-Shawarby, I., Rakha, Inman, V., and Davis, G. "Effect of Yellow-Phase Trigger Distance on Driver Behavior at High Speed Signalized Intersections." 9th International IEEE Conference on Intelligent Transportation Systems, ITSC 2006, Toronto, Canada, 2006.

  7. El-Shawarby, I., Rakha, H., Inman, V., and Davis, G. "Age and Gender Impact on Driver Behavior at the Onset of a Yellow Phase on High-Speed Signalized Intersection Approaches." Transportation Research Board Annual Meeting, Washington, DC, 2007.

  8. El-Shawarby, I., Rakha, H., Inman, V., and Davis, G. "Evaluation of Driver Deceleration Behavior at Signalized Intersections." Transportation Research Board Annual Meeting, Washington, DC, 2007.

  9. Graybiel, A. and Fregly, A.R. "A New Quantitative Ataxia Test Battery." Acta Otolaryngol JT—Acta oto-laryngologica, 61, p. 292, 1966.

  10. Kennedy, R.S., Lane, N.E., Berbaum, K.S., and Lilienthal, M.G. "Simulator Sickness Questionnaire: An Enhanced Method for Quantifying Simulator Sickness." The International Journal of Aviation Psychology, 3, p. 203, 1993.

  11. Olson, P.L. "Driver Perception Response Time." Motor Vehicle Accident Reconstruction, 67, 1989.

  12. Olson, P.L. and Sivak, M. "Perception-Response Time to Unexpected Roadway Hazards." Human Factors, 28, p. 91, 1986.

  13. Garson, D.G. PA 765: Logit, Probit, and Log-linear Models. Accessed online: August 23, 2006. (http://www2.chass.ncsu.edu/garson/pa765/logit.htm)

  14. Tabachnik, B.G. and Fidell, L.S. Using Multivariate Statistics. HarperCollins College Publishers, New York, NY, 1996.

  15. Wortman, R.H. and Matthias, J.L. "Evaluation of Driver Behavior at Signalized Intersections." Transportation Research Record, 904, p. 10, 1983.

  16. Wortman, R.H. and Matthias, J.L. An Evaluation of Driver Behavior at Signalized Intersections, Report No. FHWA/AZ-83-180, Arizona Department of Transportation, Phoenix, AZ, 1983.

  17. Setti, J.R., Rakha, H., and El-Shawarby, I. "Analysis of Brake Perception-Reaction Times on High-Speed Signalized Intersection Approaches." Transportation Research Board Annual Meeting, Washington, DC, 2007.

Federal Highway Administration | 1200 New Jersey Avenue, SE | Washington, DC 20590 | 202-366-4000
Turner-Fairbank Highway Research Center | 6300 Georgetown Pike | McLean, VA | 22101