U.S. Department of Transportation
Federal Highway Administration
1200 New Jersey Avenue, SE
Washington, DC 20590
202-366-4000


Skip to content
Facebook iconYouTube iconTwitter iconFlickr iconLinkedInInstagram

Federal Highway Administration Research and Technology
Coordinating, Developing, and Delivering Highway Transportation Innovations

 
REPORT
This report is an archived publication and may contain dated technical, contact, and link information
Back to Publication List        
Publication Number:  FHWA-HRT-15-027    Date:  November 2015
Publication Number: FHWA-HRT-15-027
Date: November 2015

 

Information As A Source of Distraction

 

Executive Summary

The overall goal of the Information as a Source of Distraction project was to further the scientific basis for decisions about the types of information that can be safely displayed within the right-of-way without adversely affecting drivers’ attention to their primary task—safe driving. There were two focus areas: electronic changeable message highway signs (CMSs) and guide signs.

This study had the following objectives:

Chapter Summary

Chapter 1, the report’s introduction, discusses the definition of distraction, the properties of modern light-emitting diode (LED) based CMSs, and the specific issues addressed in the subsequent chapters.

Chapter 2 presents some laboratory assessments of a full-color, full-matrix, LED display with 0.79-inch (20-mm) pixels. The display represents capabilities of the current generation of CMSs that are compliant with intelligent transportation system (ITS) standards. In one experiment, observers rated the visual similarity of messages on the CMS with the same messages displayed on a 60-inch (152-cm) liquid crystal display. The results of that experiment suggest that it is not necessary to emulate individual pixels of the CMS display to generate images that observers rate as reasonably similar to the CMS images. The results will have significance for future CMS research in either the Federal Highway Administration’s (FHWA) sign laboratory or the FHWA highway driving simulator.

Chapter 3 presents a study of the legibility of the CMS described in chapter 2. These results suggest that the legibility distance of the 0.79-inch (20-mm) pixel-pitch full-color display used in this test provides a longer legibility distance than the previous generation of CMS displays that used a 1.6-inch (40-mm) pixel pitch with amber LEDs.(1) They also suggest that for the display type used and drivers with approximately 20/20 vision, the 90- to 100-percentile legibility distance can be estimated using a factor of 40 to 45 ft/inch (4.8 to 5.4 m/cm) of letter height. This equates to a somewhat lower legibility distance for 0.79-inch (20-mm) pixel pitch CMS display than that provided by the 30 ft/inch (3.6 m/cm) legibility distance criterion in the Manual on Uniform Traffic Control Devices for Streets and Highways (MUTCD), assuming a 20/40 visual acuity.(2) (Because the relationship between letter height and visual acuity is linear, the MUTCD criterion would yield a 60-ft/inch (7.2-m/cm) legibility distance for individuals with 20/20 visual acuity.)

Chapter 4 examines the legibility of CMS messages in a dynamic roadway environment. In this study, observers drove toward the CMS on a winding path at 25 mi/h (40 km/h) and read the messages on the sign as soon as possible. An eye-tracker was used to assess when the observers were looking at the signs. The time and distance for the beginning and ending of response were recorded. The following properties of the messages on the sign were assessed for their effect on gaze behavior and message reading:

Chapter 5 presents an experiment conducted in the FHWA highway driving simulator to evaluate whether frequently changing overhead freeway CMS displays with human faces and colorful backgrounds would distract drivers more than static travel-time-related messages or a blank CMS. Another purpose of the experiment was to evaluate whether noncritical information presented on frequently occurring CMSs (i.e., a CMS every 0.5 mi (.8 km)) would cause drivers on a 48-min trip to lose respect for or habituate to the CMS messaging and result in drivers missing a safety-critical CMS message. The results showed that gaze behavior (i.e., number of looks, duration of looks) did not differ between signs with rapidly changing faces and static text messages. Drivers briefly looked at signs with changing faces and travel times about 40 percent of the time (40 percent of the signs) when headway to the car ahead was greater than 1.5 s. When headways were shorter than 1.5 s, the probability of briefly looking at a non-blank sign was about 0.15. Regardless of what was displayed on the CMS, the mean duration of individual looks was 0.2 s or less.

For the simulated trip, in which the drivers passed under 96 noncritical CMS message signs, there was no indication of habituation. The 97th CMS carried the message “ACCIDENT AHEAD ALL LANES BLOCKED USE NEXT EXIT.” Of the 32 participants, 9 failed to exit after passing under that sign. However, only one of the nine participants claimed to be unaware of the critical message. The majority of the remaining participants tried to exit but were unable to find a safe gap to change lanes.

Chapter 6 presents another experiment in the FHWA highway driving simulator similar to the experiment described in chapter 5. However, in this experiment, a spilled load of logs was in the participant’s lane, and the primary dependent measure was whether the participant avoided hitting the spilled load. There were 72 CMSs—1 every 0.5 mi (0.8 km), the 3 sign types (changing faces, travel-time messages, and blank) occurred in cycles of 3. The 72d sign was blank for a third of the participants, had a travel-time message for another third, and had changing faces for the final third. The spilled load came early in the trip (just before the third CMS) for half the participants and late in the trip (just before the last CMS) for the other half. Of 80 participants, 21 hit the logs. There was no significant relationship between the content of the CMS at the spill site and the probability of hitting the spilled load.

Chapter 7 examined the effect of the frequency and spacing of guide signs on navigation, eye glance behavior, and driving performance. The primary focus was the frequency and spacing of specific-service signs and supplemental guide signs. The number of supplemental guide signs varied between zero and three as did the number of specific service signs. The number of destinations on guide signs varied between one and two. The distance between the three types of guide signs also varied. In most conditions, the spacing minimum was 800 ft (244 m), which is the current standard. In the remaining two conditions, the spacing was 400 and 200ft (122 and 61 m). Overall, the results support the current MUTCD standard of 800 ft (244 m) spacing between signs and up to two destinations on advance and supplemental guide signs. There was some evidence that the specific-service six-panel logo signs used in this study required too much visual attention, especially when there was more than one specific-service sign. Participants appeared to scan food and gas service signs in search of a lodging destination and thus did not appear to use the sign legends in their search strategy.

Summary of findings and Recommendations

The overall goal of the project was to further the scientific basis for decisions about the types of information that can be displayed within the right-of-way without adversely affecting drivers’ attention to their primary task—safe driving. There were two focus areas: electronic CMSs and guide signs. Findings and recommendations include the following:

 

 

Federal Highway Administration | 1200 New Jersey Avenue, SE | Washington, DC 20590 | 202-366-4000
Turner-Fairbank Highway Research Center | 6300 Georgetown Pike | McLean, VA | 22101