U.S. Department of Transportation
Federal Highway Administration
1200 New Jersey Avenue, SE
Washington, DC 20590
202-366-4000


Skip to content
Facebook iconYouTube iconTwitter iconFlickr iconLinkedInInstagram

Federal Highway Administration Research and Technology
Coordinating, Developing, and Delivering Highway Transportation Innovations

 
REPORT
This report is an archived publication and may contain dated technical, contact, and link information
Back to Publication List        
Publication Number:  FHWA-HRT-16-061     Date:  November 2016
Publication Number: FHWA-HRT-16-061
Date: November 2016

 

Intersection Conflict Warning System Human Factors: Final Report

CHAPTER 3. METHOD

The current study was conducted to provide empirical support for recommending standard wording of ICWS signs and provide guidance on the placement of warnings on the minor road approaches. The study consisted of four parts.

In the first part, each participant viewed six animations of approaches to intersections with ICWSs. After each video, they were shown still images of the ICWS sign in the video and asked the following two questions: (1) “Please tell us what you would do in response to the sign(s) with the flashing yellow lights” and (2) “What does the warning sign mean?”

In the second part, participants were told the purpose of a particular ICWS sign and then asked to agree or disagree with statements about that sign. Each statement was rated on a seven-point scale where a rating of 1 represented “strongly agree” and a rating of 7 represented “strongly disagree.”

In the third part, participants were asked to rank alternative messages that might be displayed on the sign from the best to the least desirable. There were 9 alternative wordings for the minor approach and 10 alternative wordings for the major road approach. Each set of rankings was done twice, once with the “WHEN FLASHING” placard present and once without. After each of the four ranking exercises, participants were asked to explain what factors influenced their rankings.

In the fourth part, participants were shown images of signs or pairs of signs and asked to agree or disagree with statements concerning the meaning of the flashing beacons or the various states of the blank-out signs.

In all four parts, the stimuli were presented on a 60-inch color liquid crystal display (LCD) screen with a resolution of 1,920 horizontal by 1,080 vertical pixels. Participants were seated about 8 ft from the screen. A table with a large mouse pad and mouse were positioned in front of the participants’ chairs. Participants used a mouse to make rating and ranking responses. A research assistant transcribed verbal responses. The transcriptions were displayed on the LCD screen, and participants were asked to verify transcription accuracy. The room was dimly lit to minimize reflections on the LCD screen.

All of the signs used in this study were developed with SignCAD® software.(9) This software ensured that all the signs were compliant with the Manual on Uniform Traffic Control Devices (MUTCD) except where noted.(1) In scaling signs to the specified dimensions for a given viewing distance, the animation software used for this study can result in legibility distances that are less than would typically be experienced in the real world. To overcome this limitation, all diamond signs used in the videos were scaled to 60 inches per side. In addition, for most of the signs, FHWA E modified font was used instead of the FHWA C font that would normally be used on diamond-shaped warning signs.(10) Otherwise, the spacing of letters and words complied with the MUTCD.(1)

Upon arrival at the laboratory, participants reviewed and signed a record of informed consent, presented their valid driver’s licenses, and completed a vision test using a Bailey Lovie eye chart. After these preliminaries, they were seated in front of the LCD screen and completed the four parts of the experiment. The four parts were always administered in the same order as enumerated in the following sections.

PART 1: WHAT WOULD YOU DO AND WHAT DOES THE SIGN MEAN?

Three of the six videos featured ICWS signs on the through road approach to a stop-controlled intersection, and the remaining three featured ICWS signs for the stop-controlled approach. For each of the three through and stop-controlled approaches, one had a two-lane through road, one had a four-lane undivided through road, and one had a four-lane divided through road. Given that there were 18 different word messages to be evaluated, two different sign locations on the minor road, and warnings with and without a “WHEN FLASHING” placard, only a limited number of options could be displayed in 6 videos. To partially address this limitation, there were 12 videos, with the same 6 scenarios (3 minor road approaches and 3 major road approaches) with different combinations of wordings, placards, and static versus blank-out signs on the 2 sets of 6 videos. To further mitigate the limitation of only 12 unique videos, 6 different orders of video scenarios were used.

All of the videos were created with Autodesk InfraWorks® 360. This software enables users to download roadway alignments, elevation data, and other infrastructure information for many geographic locations. From this information, visualizations of various roadway design proposals can be created. For this study, data for the following three intersections were downloaded:

  • Buffalo Shoals Road and Shuford Road in Lincolnton, NC (a two-lane by two-lane intersection).
  • Richland Highway and Ervintown Road in Richlands, NC (a four-lane undivided by two-lane intersection).
  • U.S. 151 and Old Dubuque in Anamosa, IA (a four-lane divided by two-lane intersection).

The Buffalo Shoals/Shuford and U.S. 151/Old Dubuque intersections were selected because they had existing ICWS treatments. Videos were created for each of the four approach legs of the three intersections, and different ICWS sign/placard treatments were modeled on each approach.

The content of the six video animations shown to each of six groups of participants is described in table 7.


Table 7. Video animation content.

Approach Road Approach Direction Major Road Message Placard Road Class Blank- Out Sign Location

Group 1

Richland

North

4 L U

Entering vehicles

No

Major

No

Right

Ervintown

North

4 L U

Approaching traffic

Yes

Minor

Yes

Across

Old Dubuque

East

4 L D

Watch for approaching traffic

No

Minor

No

Missouri

Shuford

South

2 L

Cross traffic

No

Minor

Yes

Across

U.S. 151

North

4 L D

Watch for entering vehicles

Yes

Major

No

Right|Left

Buffalo Shoals

South

2 L

Crossing vehicle

Yes

Major

No

Right

Group 2

Richland

East

4 L U

Expect entering traffic

No

Major

No

Right

Buffalo Shoals

North

2 L

Crossing vehicle

Yes

Major

No

Right

US 151

South

4 L D

Traffic entering

No

Major

No

Right|Left

Shufford

North

2 L

Approaching vehicle

Yes

Minor

No

Across

Old Dubuque

West

4 L D

Traffic approaching when flashing

Yes

Minor

No

Missouri

Ervintown

South

4 L U

Expect cross traffic

No

Minor

Yes

Missouri

Group 3

Buffalo Shoals

North

2 L

Crossing vehicle

Yes

Major

No

Right

Richland

North

4 L U

Entering vehicle

No

Major

No

Right

U.S. 151

South

4 L D

Traffic entering

No

Major

No

Right|Left

Shuford

South

2 L

Cross traffic

No

Minor

Yes

Across

Old Dubuque

West

4 L D

Traffic approaching when flashing

Yes

Minor

No

Missouri

Ervintown

North

4 L U

Approaching traffic

Yes

Minor

Yes

Across

Group 4

Shuford

North

2 L

Approaching vehicle

Yes

Minor

No

Across

Old Dubuque

East

4 L D

Watch for approaching traffic

No

Minor

No

Missouri

Ervintown

South

4 L U

Expect cross traffic

No

Minor

Yes

Missouri

U.S. 151

North

4 L D

Watch for entering vehicles

Yes

Major

No

Right|Left

Buffalo Shoals

South

2 L

Crossing vehicle

Yes

Major

No

Right

Richland

East

4 L U

Expect entering traffic

No

Major

No

Right

Group 5

Old Dubuque

West

4 L D

Traffic approaching when flashing

Yes

Minor

No

Missouri

Shuford

South

2 L

Cross traffic

No

Minor

Yes

Across

Richland

East

4 L U

Expect entering traffic

No

Major

No

Right

U.S. 151

South

4 L D

Traffic entering

No

Major

No

Right|Left

Ervintown

North

4 L U

Approaching traffic

Yes

Minor

Yes

Across

Buffalo Shoals

North

2 L

Crossing vehicle

Yes

Major

No

Right

Group 6

Old Dubuque

East

4 L D

Watch for approaching traffic

No

Minor

No

Missouri

Shuford

North

2 L

Approaching vehicle

Yes

Minor

No

Across

Ervintown

South

4 L U

Expect cross traffic

No

Minor

Yes

Missouri

Buffalo Shoals

South

2 L

Crossing vehicle

Yes

Major

No

Right

Richland

North

4 L U

Entering vehicle

No

Major

No

Right

U.S. 151

North

4 L D

Watch for entering vehicles

Yes

Major

No

Right|Left

2 L = Two-lane.

4 L D = Four-lane divided.

4 L U = Four-lane undivided.


A screenshot from one of the two approaches on a two-lane through road to a two-lane stop- controlled intersection is shown in figure 3. A screenshot of one of two approaches on a four-lane divided highway to a two-lane stop-controlled intersection is shown in figure 4. One of two approaches on a four-lane undivided highway to a two-lane stop-controlled intersection is shown in figure 5. One of two stop-controlled intersections with a two-lane through road is shown in figure 6. One of two stop-controlled intersections with a four-lane undivided highway is shown in figure 7. One of two stop-controlled intersections with a four-lane divided highway is shown in figure 8.


Figure 3. Screenshot. Blank-out sign with “WHEN FLASHING” placard along two-lane approach to a stop-controlled cross street. This screenshot features a shot of a two-lane undivided road. There is a black diamond sign on the right side of the road that reads, “CROSS TRAFFIC AHEAD.” The diamond sign has beacons on both sides, and one beacon is lit. Below the diamond sign is a square yellow placard that reads, “WHEN FLASHING.”

Figure 3. Screenshot. Blank-out sign with “WHEN FLASHING” placard along two-lane approach to a stop-controlled cross street.


Figure 4. Screenshot. ICWS signs on four-lane divided highway approach. This screenshot features one-half (two lanes) of a four-lane divided highway. There are identical intersection conflict warning system setups on both sides of the lanes. The setups include a yellow diamond sign that reads, “WATCH FOR ENTERING VEHICLE,” beacons on both sides of the diamond sign (one of which is lit), and a square yellow placard below the diamond sign that reads, “WHEN FLASHING.”

Figure 4. Screenshot. ICWS signs on four-lane divided highway approach.


Figure 5. Screenshot. ICWS sign on four-lane undivided highway approach. This screenshot features a four-lane undivided highway with a two-way center turn lane separating the two sides of the highway. On the right side of the highway is the intersection conflict warning system. It has a yellow diamond sign that reads, “ENTERING VEHICLES.” There are beacons on both sides of the diamond sign (one of which is lit), and a gas station and a residential home are visible behind the yellow diamond sign.

Figure 5. Screenshot. ICWS sign on four-lane undivided highway approach.


Figure 6. Screenshot. ICWS warning at intersection with two-lane highway. This screenshot features a minor road intersecting with a two-lane highway, and it is situated from the perspective of the minor road. There is a stop sign at the minor road. On the other side of the intersection (e.g., the corner diagonal from the stop sign), there is an intersection conflict warning system with a black diamond sign that reads “CROSS TRAFFIC.” There are beacons on both sides of the sign, and one beacon is lit.

Figure 6. Screenshot. ICWS warning at intersection with two-lane highway.


Figure 7. Screenshot. ICWS warning at stop-controlled intersection with four-lane undivided highway. This screenshot features a minor road intersecting with a four-lane undivided highway and is situated from the perspective of the minor road. On the left side of the minor road, there is an intersection conflict warning system with a black diamond sign that reads “EXPECT CROSS TRAFFIC.” There are lit beacons above and below the sign. The top beacon is facing the minor road, and the bottom beacon is facing the highway traffic coming from the right of the intersection. There is a car approaching the intersection on the highway from the left.

Figure 7. Screenshot. ICWS warning at stop-controlled intersection with four-lane undivided highway.


Figure 8. Screenshot. ICWS at stop-controlled approach to four-lane divided highway. This screenshot features a minor road intersecting with a four-lane divided highway and is situated from the perspective of the minor road. On the left side of the dividing media, there is a one-way sign (pointing right) and an intersection conflict warning system (ICWS) with a yellow diamond sign that reads, “WATCH FOR APPROACHING TRAFFIC,” and it has beacons on both sides (one of which is lit). On the right side of the dividing media, there is a triangular yield sign and a one-way sign pointing left. On the right corner of the far side of the intersection, there is an ICWS with a yellow diamond sign that reads “WATCH FOR APPROACHING TRAFFIC,” and it has beacons on both sides (one of which is lit).

Figure 8. Screenshot. ICWS at stop-controlled approach to four-lane divided highway.


PART 2: ICWS SIGN PERCEPTIONS

In an effort to assess perceptions and attitudes regarding ICWS, participants were presented with 21 statements concerning an ICWS sign and asked to agree or disagree with the statement using a 7-point scale. Before providing these ratings, the participant was given a brief explanation of the sign used in connection with each of the statements. There were two explanations. The minor road explanation is shown in figure 9, and the explanation for the major road warning signs was “This sign is used on a major road to alert drivers that a vehicle has been detected that is about to enter the highway from a cross street.” For any particular participant, the same warning sign message was shown next to each of the 21 statements. Different messages were each of six participant groups. The minor road messages were as follows:

  • “ENTERING VEHICLES.”
  • “EXPECT ENTERING TRAFFIC.”
  • “CROSSING VEHICLE.”

The major road messages were as follows:

  • “APPROACHING VEHICLE.”
  • “TRAFFIC APPROACHING WHEN FLASHING.”
  • “WATCH FOR APPROACHING TRAFFIC.”

Figure 9. Screenshot. Explanation of ICWS sign on minor road approach. This screenshot features an intersection conflict warning system with a yellow diamond sign that reads, “WATCH FOR APPROACHING TRAFFIC.” It has beacons on both sides (one of which is lit). Below the sign is a black rectangular bar containing the words “Watch for Approaching Traffic.” Below the black bar is a paragraph of text that reads, “This sign is used on minor roads to warn drivers at a stop sign that vehicles on the highway are approaching the intersection. As a vehicle on the highway approaches the cross street, the flashers are activated to alert drivers approaching the stop sign about traffic they may not be able to see (for instance, because of trees, hills or buildings), but that will be crossing the intersection shortly.”

Figure 9. Screenshot. Explanation of ICWS sign on minor road approach.

None of the signs shown in this part were accompanied by a “WHEN FLASHING” placard. There were 6 different orders for the 21 statements that were evenly distributed across participants.

Figure 10 shows an example of how the statements were presented. Participants made their ratings by using a mouse to click on one of the seven numbers on the left half of the screen. The statements were as follows:

  • We need signs like this.
  • This warning will make some drivers overconfident.
  • All drivers should understand this warning.
  • It would be hard to use the information this sign provides.
  • This warning may be confusing to some people.
  • This sign is just more clutter on the highway.
  • Signs like this are distracting.
  • This warning would make me a safer driver.
  • This warning will prevent crashes.
  • This warning would be unreliable.
  • This sign would get my attention.
  • This warning is too complicated.
  • This warning is totally unnecessary.
  • I would trust this warning.
  • Other drivers will ignore this sign.
  • If this sign is NOT flashing, I do NOT need to watch for traffic.
  • I don’t know how I should respond to this warning.
  • I would ignore this warning.
  • This sign does NOT provide enough information.
  • This warning is easy to understand.
  • When this sign is flashing, I need to be more alert for danger.

Figure 10. Screenshot. Example of screen used for obtaining agreement ratings. This screenshot features a yellow diamond sign that reads, “WATCH FOR APPROACHING TRAFFIC,” and it has beacons on both sides (one of which is lit). At the top of the screenshot is the statement, “We need signs like this.” Below that statement are the numbers 1 through 7 and a corresponding statement for each number as follows: 1: Strongly Agree, 2: Agree, 3: Somewhat Agree, 4: Neither Agree or Disagree, 5: Somewhat Disagree, 6: Disagree, and 7: Strongly Disagree.

Figure 10. Screenshot. Example of screen used for obtaining agreement ratings.


The statements were organized around a few themes so that a factor analysis of the ratings could potentially reveal something about the mental model participants had of the ICWS. These themes were as follows:

  • Does the sign command respect?
  • Is the sign understandable?
  • Is the sign viewed as safety related?
  • Will the sign capture attention?
  • Is the sign trustworthy?

A balance was sought between questions concerning how the participant would react (e.g., I would trust this warning) and how the participant thought other drivers would react (e.g., other drivers will ignore this sign). A balance was also sought in questions with a positive valance (e.g., we need signs like this) and a negative valance (e.g., signs like this are distracting).


PART 3: PREFERENCE RANKING

In this portion of the experiment, participants were asked to rank the alternative wordings on ICWS signs from most preferred to least preferred. There were 9 alternative wordings for the minor road approaches and 10 alternative wordings for the major road approaches. The ranking was preceded by one of the signs and an explanation of the purpose of the sign (the same explanations as provided in part 2). The rankings were performed four times for each approach (major and minor) and with and without the “WHEN FLASHING” placard. Alternatives that included “when flashing” on the diamond sign were always displayed without the placard.

Figure 11 provides an example of the display used to elicit the rankings. A letter was displayed beneath images of signs with each wording. Above the images, on the left side of the screen, were tiles that each had a letter of the alphabet. Participants were asked to use a mouse to drag the tiles from the left side of the screen to the right side and order the letters from top to bottom according to their preference. The initial order of letter tiles was randomized for each participant. Six different orders of the signs, evenly distributed across participants, were displayed across the bottom of the screen.


Figure 11. Screenshot. Example of rating screen for nine alternative minor road warnings. This screenshot shows a rating screen for nine alternative minor road warnings. At the top of the screen is the text, “You are on a two lane road. There is a major road ahead. Beacons begin to flash on a warning sign. The sign warns you that there is a driver on the major road that may be entering or crossing the highway. Which of these signs do you think best communicates the message?” At the bottom of the screen are nine yellow diamond signs with various intersection conflict warning system messages. In the middle of the screen are instructions for the user on how to rank the signs in order of preference.

Figure 11. Screenshot. Example of rating screen for nine alternative minor road warnings.


The wording choices for minor roads were as follows:

  • “EXPECT CROSS TRAFFIC.”
  • “APPROACHING TRAFFIC.”
  • “ENTERING TRAFFIC.”
  • “TRAFFIC APPROACHING WHEN FLASHING.”
  • “APPROACHING VEHICLE.”
  • “VEHICLES APPROACHING.”
  • “WATCH FOR APPROACHING TRAFFIC.”
  • “CROSS TRAFFIC.”
  • “WATCH FOR APPROACHING VEHICLES.”

The wording choices for major roads were:

  • “EXPECT ENTERING TRAFFIC.”
  • “CROSSING TRAFFIC.”
  • “TRAFFIC ENTERING WHEN FLASHING.”
  • “ENTERING VEHICLES.”
  • “TRAFFIC ENTERING.”
  • “VEHICLE CROSSING.”
  • “WATCH FOR ENTERING VEHICLE.”
  • “WATCH FOR ENTERING TRAFFIC.”
  • “CROSSING VEHICLE.”
  • “CROSS TRAFFIC AHEAD.”

Immediately after each ranking exercise, participants were asked to explain why they ranked the signs the way they did. Their verbal responses were transcribed by the researcher. An example of the screen displayed while participants explained their preferences is shown in figure 12.


Figure 12. Screenshot. Example of screen shown while participants were asked to explain their preferences. This screenshot shows 10 ranked yellow diamond signs with various intersection conflict warning system messages. At the top of the screenshot is the statement, “Please tell us about why you ranked the signs the way you did.”

Figure 12. Screenshot. Example of screen shown while participants were asked to explain their preferences.


PART 4: COMPREHENSION

Some agencies use the “WHEN FLASHING” (W16-13) placard with ICWS signs while others do not.(1) The placard is intended to indicate the immediacy of the warning when the beacons are flashing. However, some agencies are concerned that motorists might interpret the signs to mean that no hazard is present when the beacons are not flashing. One goal of the questions in this section was to provide an estimate of how participants interpret the presence or absence of the “WHEN FLASHING” placard. The questions addressing this issue are shown in figure 13 through figure 15.


Figure 13. Screenshot. Screen examining comprehension of ICWS signs with placard when the beacons are flashing. This screenshot shows two side-by-side intersection conflict warning system yellow diamond signs that read, “WATCH FOR ENTERING VEHICLE,” that have beacons on both sides (one of which is lit for each sign). Below the diamond sign on the right is a square yellow placard that reads, “WHEN FLASHING.” At the top of the screen, there is a statement that reads, “When flashing, these signs mean the same thing.” At the left side of the screen, there are buttons for the user to press to indicate whether they agree or disagree with that statement.

Figure 13. Screenshot. Screen examining comprehension of ICWS signs with placard when the beacons are flashing.


Figure 14. Screenshot. Screen examining comprehension of ICWS signs with placard when the beacons are not flashing. This screenshot shows two side-by-side intersection conflict warning system yellow diamond signs that read, “WATCH FOR ENTERING VEHICLE,” that have beacons on both sides (none of which are lit). Below the diamond sign on the right is a square yellow placard that reads, “WHEN FLASHING.” At the top of the screen, there is a statement that reads, “When not flashing, these signs mean the same thing.” At the left side of the screen, there are buttons for the user to press to indicate whether they agree or disagree with that statement.

Figure 14. Screenshot. Screen examining comprehension of ICWS signs with placard when the beacons are not flashing.


Figure 15. Screenshot. Screen examining whether driver would check for traffic when beacons are not flashing. This screenshot shows an intersection conflict warning system with a yellow diamond sign that reads, “WATCH FOR ENTERING VEHICLE,” that has beacons on both sides (neither of which are lit). Below the yellow diamond sign is a square yellow placard that reads, “WHEN FLASHING.” At the top of the screen, there is a statement that reads, “When the beacons are flashing, it is not necessary to look for approaching traffic.” At the left of the screen, there are buttons for the user to press to indicate whether they agree or disagree with that statement.

Figure 15. Screenshot. Screen examining whether driver would check for traffic when beacons are not flashing.


Some agencies use blank-out signs for ICWS signage. The main intent of using a blank-out sign is to display no message at all when the beacons cannot function, such as during a power outage. Another goal of this part of the study was to provide an estimate of how the participants interpret ICWS blank-out signs when they are blank. Figure 16 through figure 21 show the screens used to assess comprehension of blank-out ICWS signs.


Figure 16. Screenshot. Screen used to explain why blank-out signs are used. This screenshot features a statement at the top that reads, “Some agencies use blank-out signs. The messages on these signs are formed with illuminated lights so that when the power is off, there is no message.” Below that statement are side-by-side intersection conflict warning systems with black diamond signs. The left sign reads, “EXPECT CROSS TRAFFIC,” and has two beacons above and below (the bottom two of which are lit). The right sign features the same setup but has no message or lit beacons. The left sign is labeled “Power on,” and the right sign is labeled “Power off.”

Figure 16. Screenshot. Screen used to explain why blank-out signs are used.


Figure 17. Screenshot. Screen used to assess comprehension of blank-out message when beacons are active. This screenshot shows an intersection conflict warning system with a black diamond sign that reads, “EXPECT CROSS TRAFFIC,” that has two beacons above and below it (the bottom two of which are lit). At the top of the screen, there is a statement that reads, “Cross traffic is present.” At the left side of the screen, there are buttons for the user to press to indicate whether they agree or disagree with that statement.

Figure 17. Screenshot. Screen used to assess comprehension of blank-out message when beacons are active.


Figure 18. Screenshot. Screen used to assess blank-out sign understanding when the beacons are not active. This screenshot shows an intersection conflict warning system with a black diamond sign that reads, “EXPECT CROSS TRAFFIC,” that has two beacons above and below it (none of which are lit). At the top of the screen, there is a statement that reads, “Cross traffic is not present.” At the left side of the screen, there are buttons for the user to press to indicate whether they agree or disagree with that statement.

Figure 18. Screenshot. Screen used to assess blank-out sign understanding when the beacons are not active.


Figure 19. Screenshot. Screen used to asses blank-out sign understanding when the sign is blank. This screenshot features a statement at the top that reads, “Today, there is no traffic approaching.” Below that statement are side-by-side intersection conflict warning systems with black diamond signs. The left sign reads, “EXPECT CROSS TRAFFIC,” that has two beacons above and below (the bottom two of which are lit). The right sign features the same setup but has no message or lit beacons. The left sign is labeled “Flashing,” and the right sign is labeled “Not Flashing.” At the left side of the screen, there are buttons for the user to press to indicate whether they agree or disagree with that statement.

Figure 19. Screenshot. Screen used to asses blank-out sign understanding when the sign is blank.


Figure 20. Screenshot. Screen used to assess interpretation of a blank-out sign when the sign is on but the beacons are not active. This screenshot shows an intersection conflict warning system with a black diamond sign that reads, “EXPECT CROSS TRAFFIC,” that has two beacons above and below it (none of which are lit). The sign is labeled “Not Flashing.” At the top of the screen, there is a statement that reads, “No traffic is coming on the crossroad.” At the left side of the screen, there are buttons for the user to press to indicate whether they agree or disagree with that statement.

Figure 20. Screenshot. Screen used to assess interpretation of a blank-out sign when the sign is on but the beacons are not active.


Figure 21. Screenshot. Second screen used to assess interpretation of a blank-out sign when the sign is on but the beacons are not active. This screenshot shows an intersection conflict warning system with a black diamond sign that reads, “EXPECT CROSS TRAFFIC,” that has two beacons above and below it (none of which are lit). At the top of the screen, there is a statement that reads, “No need to look for traffic when the beacons are not flashing.” At the left side of the screen, there are buttons for the user to press to indicate whether they agree or disagree with that statement.

Figure 21. Screenshot. Second screen used to assess interpretation of a blank-out sign when the sign is on but the beacons are not active.


The final question in this section was intended to obtain a preference for the location of ICWS warning signs for minor road drivers at four-lane divided highways. This question asked whether the warning would be most effective if the signs were placed in a location close to where the approaching vehicles were located or directly across from the driver when looking forward at the stop sign. All of the participants had previously seen video animations that used both placement locations. Before asking which location they thought would be more effective, participants were shown short video clips with exemplars of each placement. Figure 22 shows the placement across from the stop line. Figure 23 shows the upstream placement. Figure 24 shows the screen that participants used to indicate their preference among the two setups.


Figure 22. Screenshot. Exemplar from video showing the ICWS sign placement across from the stop and yield lines. This screenshot features a minor road intersecting with a four-lane divided highway and is situated from the perspective of the minor road. On the right side of the intersection is a stop sign, a one-way sign (pointing right), and a square placard that reads, “DIVIDED HIGHWAY,” with arrows indicating the layout of the intersection. On the left side of the dividing media, there is a one-way sign (pointing right) and an intersection conflict warning system (ICWS) with a yellow diamond sign that reads, “WATCH FOR APPROACHING TRAFFIC,” that has beacons on both sides (one of which is lit). On the right side of the dividing media, there is a triangular yield sign and a one-way sign pointing left. On the right corner of the far side of the intersection, there is an ICWS with a yellow diamond sign that reads “WATCH FOR APPROACHING TRAFFIC,” that has beacons on both sides (one of which is lit).

Figure 22. Screenshot. Exemplar from video showing the ICWS sign placement across from the stop and yield lines.


Figure 23. Screenshot. Exemplar from video showing the ICWS sign placements up stream on the cross road. This screenshot shows a minor road intersecting with one-half (two lanes) of a divided highway and is situated from the perspective of the minor road. On the left corner of the intersection is an intersection conflict warning system with a yellow diamond sign that reads “WATCH FOR APPROACHING TRAFFIC,” that has two beacons above and below the sign (the bottom two of which are lit). Across from the minor road is a black triangular sign and a one-way sign (pointing right).

Figure 23. Screenshot. Exemplar from video showing the ICWS sign placements up stream on the cross road.


Figure 24. Screenshot. Screen from which participants indicated which sign placement they thought would be most effective. This screenshot features two side-by-side depictions of a minor road intersecting with a four-lane divided highway. The left picture (labeled as “A”) shows STOP and WATCH FOR APPROACHING TRAFFIC signs. At the top of the screen is the statement, “Which location do you think would be most effective in alerting you to the fact that conflicting traffic is approaching?” At the bottom of the screens are buttons for the user to push to indicate whether they prefer location A or location B.

Figure 24. Screenshot. Screen from which participants indicated which sign placement they thought would be most effective.


PARTICIPANTS

Out of 189 participants, 96 participants were over 46 years old. Among the older participants, 48 were male, and 48 were female. Among the younger participants, 47 were male, and 46 were female. All participants possessed a valid driver’s license and had a visual acuity of 20/40 or better. All participants resided in the Washington, DC, metropolitan area, mostly in Northern Virginia. The mean age of male participants was 47 years old (range 19 to 78 years old), and the mean age of female participants was 45 years old (range 18 to 79 years old). All participants completed an informed consent form prior to beginning. The choice of age 46 as the criterion for splitting young from old age groups was somewhat arbitrary but still based on the fact that the median age database from which participants were recruited was 46 years old.

 

 

 

Federal Highway Administration | 1200 New Jersey Avenue, SE | Washington, DC 20590 | 202-366-4000
Turner-Fairbank Highway Research Center | 6300 Georgetown Pike | McLean, VA | 22101